Steel Flanges Trading, Exporting, pipe flange, forged flange, carbon steel flange, manufacturer, exporter, Shandong HyupShin Flanges Co., Ltd flange, pipe flange, forged flange, carbon steel flange, manufacturer, exporter, Shandong HyupShin Flanges Co., Ltd
  • Pipe flanges manufacturer
  • Forged Flanges Company
  • Carbon Steel flanges factory
  • Flanges DIN, ANSI, EN1092-1, JIS, BS4504
  • Steel Flanges Exporter, Supplier
  • 1
  • 2
  • 3
  • 4
  • 5
Home > Flange's News > Flanges > View  
Flanges

Flange color differences of brood parasitic brown-headed cowbirds from nests of two host species.

Views:   Post Date: 2/3/2013 5:28:52 AM

Evidence of recognition and discrimination of parasitic nestlings is relatively rare among hosts of avian brood parasite species (Redondo 1993; Grim et al. 2003; Langmore et al. 2003, 2009; Schuetz 2005b; Sato et al. 2010; Shizuka and Lyon 2010). Patterns of parasite chick's visual and/or acoustic similarity of host nestlings in a handful of brood parasite lineages (Anderson et al. 2009, Sato et al. 2010, Langmore et al. 2011) imply mimicry to avoid rejection (Langmore et al. 2003, Payne 2005, Tokue and Ueda 2010, Langmore et al. 2011). Hosts may discriminate not only by directly rejecting foreign nestlings, but by providing better care or higher quality prey (Schuetz 2005a, Soler 2008) for nestlings with particular attributes (Rothstein 1978, Lichtenstein 2001, Dugas 2009), resulting in variation among nestlings in growth rate and condition (Hauber and Kilner 2007). Nestling discrimination among cowbird hosts has been documented for Rufousbellied Thrushes (Turdus rufiventris) parasitized by the non-evicting generalist Shiny Cowbird (Molothrus bonariensis) (Lichtenstein 2001), but is not yet known to occur in any hosts of the Brown-headed Cowbird (M. ater). Hosts may recognize parasitic nestlings using variation in size, color, vocalization, brood size, and length of time before fledging (Langmore et al. 2003, Schuetz 2005b, Grim 2007). Variable coloration of both gapes and rictal flanges may have a signaling function, conveying nestling identity, need, health or other indicators of quality (Thorogood et al. 2008, Dugas 2010), which would need to be matched by parasitic nestlings (Nicolai 1974, Payne 2005, Hauber and Kilner 2007). flange color is typically monomorphic within species, but Brown-headed Cowbird nestlings appear polymorphic across the species so that a nestling has either distinctly yellow or white flanges with few intermediates (Rothstein 1978). Polymorphic flange color occurs in only two phylogenetically distant New World oscine genera, Geospiza and Molothrus. It is plausible this polymorphism in cowbirds is the outcome of selection for preferential parasitism of certain host species by female cowbirds to match the hostspecific flange color by the parasitic nestling (Ellison et al. 2007). Alternatively, the differences in human-perceived flange phenotype of cowbird nestlings may not result from genetic polymorphism, but from differences in carotenoid consumption and provisioning to chicks across different hosts. Carotenoid pigments are derived entirely from diet, and their concentration is known to modulate nestling mouth color (Thorogood et al. 2008). Carotenoid concentration is widely hypothesized to indicate nestling quality, as demonstrated in House Sparrows (Passer domesticus) (Loiseau et al. 2008) and Barn Swallows (Hirundo rustica) (Saino et al. 2000, 2003). We investigated whether cowbird nestlings differ in flange coloration when reared by one of two host species, Song Sparrows (Melospiza melodia) and Yellow Warblers (Setophaga petechia) using quantitative measures of coloration. Birds have a fourth violet- or ultraviolet-sensitive photoreceptor type, and human color perception is an insufficient proxy for avian color perception (Cuthill et al. 2000). We provide the first objective assessment of cowbird nestling flange colors based on measures of color using digital photographs and imaging software (Dale 2000). However, this remains a preliminary analysis because imaging software is designed for human vision and has limited value for avian perceptual studies (Stevens et al. 2007). The objective of our study was to test the hypothesis that rictal flange color of host and cowbird nestlings varies between nestlings and parasites of two sympatric hosts. We predicted measures of flange colors would differ among: (1) nestlings of different species (Yellow Warbler, Song Sparrow, and Brown-headed Cowbird), and (2) cowbird nestlings in nests of different host species. METHODS Study Site and Species.--This study was conducted in the riparian corridors of four tributaries of Mono Lake (38[degrees] 1' N, 119[degrees] 3' W) on the eastern slope of the Sierra Nevada, California, USA: Lee Vining, Mill, Rush, and Wilson creeks. We located Song Sparrow and Yellow Warbler nests and monitored nests during the 2004 breeding season following Martin and Geupel (1993) and Ralph et al. (1993). The ranges of nest initiation dates for Yellow Warbler and Song Sparrows were similar in 2004 (Tonra et al. 2009). Nestling Photographs.--We photographed nestlings on day 6 (hatching day = day 0) of the cowbird nestling cycle. This day was chosen because it coincided with the age at which nestlings are sufficiently large to band and allowed for incorporation of carotenoid pigments from host-provisioned diet into tissues. Photographs were taken with a Hewlett Packard Photosmart 215 digital camera, set to ISO 200 and 'fine' quality (1,280 x 960 pixels). We photographed all cowbirds in each nest and, if hosts were present, we also randomly selected one individual to photograph. Sixteen Brown-headed Cowbird, three Yellow Warbler, and three Song Sparrow nestlings were included in the analysis. Both cowbird and host chicks were photographed in two Song Sparrow and two Yellow Warbler nests, and one Yellow Warbler and two Song Sparrow nests each contained two cowbird nestlings. Each photograph was taken of the right side of the head against a background of gray paper with a strip composed of six l-[cm.sup.2] sections cut from paint store color sample cards (red, blue, green, white, yellow, and black) (Home Depot, Reno, NV, USA) as a color standard, and stored in a dark box between photography sessions. This allowed us to make direct comparisons of colors under varying light conditions in the field. The photographs were saved and subsequently analyzed as jpeg images using the histogram function in Adobe Photoshop Elements 8.0 (Adobe Systems Inc., San Jose, CA, USA). Storing images as jpeg compresses both image and color data (Stevens et al. 2007); color compression in this software obscures rather than enhances differences in color and would result in our failure to reject the null hypothesis (despite its falsehood: Type II statistical error). Thus, use of jpeg images made our analyses more conservative. flanges were divided into three portions for color measurement; A at the apex of the flange, site B at the fleshy middle, and site C at its most rostral point. Three replicate measures of red, green, and blue values were made at each flange site for each nestling. Red, green, and blue measures represent the intensity levels (saturation) for 24-bit color; these measures range in intensity from 0 (black) to 255 (white, totally saturated color). Measurements were also made from the center of the yellow standard present in each photograph to allow for direct comparison of the standard and biological colors. Data Analysis.--We compared red (R), green (G), and blue (B) values, while accounting for variation in light conditions associated with field work in each photograph, by first scaling according to the RGB values of the yellow standard in that photograph i. Thus, [F.sub.I]/[Y.sub.I] x 100, where I = R, G, or B value, [F.sub.I] = flange color value for i, and [Y.sub.1] = the yellow standard for i. Repeated measures at each flange site within color group (R, G, and B) were averaged across replicates. Data were analyzed using three separate univariate analyses of variance (Mixed Effects ANOVA), where each color value was the dependent variable, nestling species (Yellow Warbler, Song Sparrow, or Brown-headed Cowbird) and cowbird nestling host species (Yellow Warbler or Song Sparrow) were fixed effects, and nestling metal-band ID and flange site were random effects. Color variables R, G, and B are necessarily correlated and we also used principal components analysis (PCA) to recombine color variables into uncorrelated scores describing brightness and chromaticity (following Endler and Thery 1996). Mean PC scores were compared among host nestling species and cowbird nestling species groups using ANOVA. All analyses were conducted in JMP Version 8.0 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). [FIGURE 1 OMITTED] RESULTS Color values were significantly different among nestling species in all three univariate analyses of red, green, and blue color components (R: [F.sub.2,48] = 11.67, P < 0.0001; G: [F.sub.2,50] = 11.14, P < 0.0001; B: [F.sub.2,50] = 16.61, P < 0.0001), but not between cowbird nestlings of different host species (R: [F.sub.l,17] = 0.84, P = 0.543; G: [F.sub.1,16] = 0.12, P = 0.737; B: [F.sub.1,17] = 1.06, P = 0.319; Fig. 1). The first two Principal Components (PC) together explained 97% of the variation in nestling flange color (Table 1). The first Principal Component (PC I) explained 84% of variance in the combined RGB variables and described variation in brightness for all three color components, as implied by the consistently positive eigenvalue loadings of all three red (0.575), green (0.613), and blue (0.541) chromatic values. PC I differed across nestling species ([F.sub.2,47] = 24.96, P < 0.0001), but did not differ between cowbird nestlings from different host species nests ([F.sub.1,16] = 0.004, P = 0.950). Principal Component II (PC II) explained 14% of variance and described the color content or chromaticity of the RGB variables (i.e., hue and saturation), where blue was a positively loaded (0.793) eigenvector and red was negatively loaded (-0.591). Green was also negatively loaded, although this value was low (-0.145). Higher values of PC II reflected higher values for blue and lower values for red. PC II did not differ by nestling species ([F.sub.2,56] = 0.84, P = 0.436), but differed significantly between cowbird nestlings from different host species nests ([F.sub.1,19] = 5.89, P = 0.026) (Fig. 2). DISCUSSION flange colors of two species serving as hosts for Brown-headed Cowbird nestlings were different in most univariate and PC analyses of physical coloration measures, confirming the assumptions of species-specificity of these visual traits. Univariate measures of flange colors revealed no differences between cowbird nestlings from the two host species' nests. However, most univariate analyses of color traits are confounded with variation in brightness (Endler and Thery 1996). PC analysis, in contrast, revealed differences in PC II describing the relative saturation of red and blue among cowbird nestlings. Thus, parasitic offspring in nests of the two sympatric host species differ in physical measures of coloration, as recorded on digital photographs of live nestlings in the field. These differences may be behaviorally relevant for host discrimination, as recent studies of hosts of brood parasitic birds have demonstrated the avian-specific role of particular chromatic elements influencing egg rejection behavior (Honza et al. 2007; Cassey et al. 2008, 2009). The host-specificity of the chromatic variation of cowbird nestling flange color detected suggests a potential signaling function in the context of parasitic nestling discrimination. A mechanism may exist by which female cowbirds preferentially parasitize hosts whose nestling flange color matches that of their own chicks, despite the vast diversity of cowbird host species (Friedmann 1929). This mechanism may be similar to female parasites' host-specific egg color matching, documented in Common Cuckoos (Cuculus canorus) (Cherry et al. 2007, Aviles 2008). [FIGURE 2 OMITTED] The ontogenetic basis of host-parasite flange color matching requires additional study, as it may involve genetic and maternal effects of host-specific parasitism by female cowbirds. Alternatively, it may be the result of host-specific plasticity of cowbird chicks' coloration during development. Experimental manipulation of cowbird nestling flange color, and cross-fostering studies of parasitic eggs between different host species should provide experimental tests of some of these alternatives, as demonstrated in the host-specific begging call matching by cuckoos of Australian songbirds (Langmore et al. 2008). The difference in chromatic reflectance of flange colors (PC II) across cowbird nestlings from the two hosts may also be a developmental result of carotenoid concentration based on the diets of our two host species. Carotenoids in birds are derived entirely from diet, and are known to modulate variation in nestling mouth color by blocking the reflectance of short-wavelength (blue/green) light and cause the mouthparts to appear orange, yellow or reddish (Thorogood et al. 2008). Thus, nestlings with carotenoid-rich mouthparts are favored in species where parents exhibit a feeding preference based on nestling mouth coloration (Dugas 2009). Nestling flanges are significantly brighter, more UV reflective, and more chroma rich in the part of the flange visible during begging than hidden parts (the sides), lending support for the signaling hypothesis in the function of flange coloration (Dugas 2010). However, measurements in our study were made on the sides of the flange, and would be less visible during begging. Differences in the coloration of these two regions may be the combined result of signaling during begging and decreasing conspicuousness when not, resulting in decreased UV reflectance on sides compared to insides (Dugas 2010). The inside and side flange colors were weakly correlated (Dugas 2010), but we might expect to find even better color matching based on the inner part of the flanges. Future studies would benefit from comparing these two regions using UV-sensitive spectrophotometric measures as opposed to the human-visible only color measurements available to us from digital photographs. We did not measure ambient light at the nest. Thus, our results of species-specific differences in the color variables contributing to PC I and host-specific differences in the relative amount of blue reflectance of cowbird nestlings (PC II) may be an adaptive result of nestlings optimizing their own detection in the specific ambient light environments of the different host species' nests (Ficken 1965). Nestlings in dark nests increase conspicuousness through the relative color and size of the flange (Kilner and Davies 1998), and open-nesting species show higher achromatic contrast with the nest than cavity-nesting species (Aviles et al. 2008). Both Yellow Warblers and Song Sparrows nest in open cups, albeit typically at different heights and in different plant substrates (Tonra et al. 2009), and differences in nestling coloration based on detection under ambient light conditions are an unlikely causal explanation for the pattern of differences in cowbird flange color diversity. Our results support predictions of the hostparasite flange color matching hypothesis for Brown-headed Cowbirds and their hosts. Further experimentation is necessary to examine the role of flange color in the host-brood parasite coevolutionary arms race, including nestling mimicry and discrimination (Fraga 1998, Lichtenstein 2001). These studies have the potential to shed light on the evolution of host use strategies in cowbirds, revealing the extent to which specialization may exist in individual cowbird females. This information would have important and broadly applicable implications for both coevolutionary theory and conservation. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We are grateful to the University of California, Berkeley, Miller Institute for Basic Research in Science, Marin Rod and Gun Club Scholarship for Graduate Research in Wildlife, Humboldt State University Foundation, Mono Basin Bird Chautauqua Bird Research Grant, Human Frontier Science Program, and the PSC-CUNY Faculty Grant program for funding. M. D. Johnson, M. A. Colwell, and T. L. George provided support towards the completion of this study. We thank the Mono Lake Committee for providing logistical support in the field. This is PRBO Contribution # 1841. Received 25 January 2011. Accepted 21 July 2011. LITERATURE CITED ANDERSON, M. G., H. A. ROSS, D. H. BRUNTON, AND M. E. HAUBER. 2009. Begging call matching between a specialist brood parasite and its host: a comparative approach to detect co-evolution. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 98:208-216. AVILES, J. M. 2008. Egg colour mimicry in the Common Cuckoo Cuculus canorus as revealed by modeling host retinal function. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 275:2345-2352. AVILES, J. M., T. PEREZ-CONTRERAS, C. NAVARRO, AND J. J. SOLER. 2008. Dark nests and conspicuousness in color patterns of nestlings of altricial birds. American Naturalist 171:327-338. CASSEY, P., M. HONZA, T. GRIM, AND M. E. HAUBER. 2008. The modeling of avian visual perception predicts behavioural rejection responses to foreign egg colours. Biology Letters 4:515-517. CASSEY, P., J. G. EWEN, N. J. MARSHALL, M. VOROBYEV, T. M. BLACKBURN, AND M. E. HAUBER. 2009. Are avian eggshell colours effective intraspecific communication signals in the Muscicapoidea? A perceptual modeling approach. Ibis 151:689-698. CHERRY, M. I., A. T. D. BENNETT, AND C. MOSKAT. 2007. Do cuckoos choose nests of Great Reed Warblers on the basis of host egg appearance? Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20:1218-1222. CUTHILL, I. C., J. C. PARTRIDGE, A. T. D. BENNETT, S. C. CHURCH, N. S. HART, AND S. HUNT. 2000. Ultraviolet vision in birds. Advances in the Study of Behavior 29:159-213. DALE, J. 2000. Ornamental plumage does not signal male quality in Red-billed Queleas. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 267:2143-2149. DUGAS, M. B. 2009. House sparrow, Passer domesticus, parents preferentially feed nestling with mouth colours that appear carotenoid-rich. Animal Behaviour 78: 767-772. DUGAS, M. B. 2010. Nestling birds put their best flange forward. Journal of Avian Biology 41:336-341. ELLISON, K., S. G. SEALY, AND H. R. MCGAHA. 2007. Color variation among nestling Brown-headed Cowbirds (Molothrus ater) does not reflect differential success with hosts in Texas. Auk 124:526 536. ENDLER, J. A. AND M. THERY. 1996. Interacting effects of lek placement, display behavior, ambient light, and color patterns in three neotropical forest-dwelling birds. American Naturalist 148:421M52. FICKEN, M. S. 1965. Mouth color of nestling passerines and its use in taxonomy. Wilson Bulletin 77:71-75. FRAGA, R. M. 1998. Interactions of the parasitic Screaming Cowbirds (Molothrus rufoaxillaris and M. bonaerensis) with a shared host, the Bay-winged Cowbird (Molothrus badius). Pages 173-193 in Parasitic birds and their hosts: studies in coevolution (S. I. Rothstein and S. K. Robinson, Editors). Oxford University Press, New York, USA. FREIDMANN, H. 1929. The cowbirds: a study in the biology of social parasitism. Charles C. Thomas, Springfield, Illinois, USA. GRIM, T. 2007. Experimental evidence for chick discrimination without recognition in a brood parasite host. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 274:373-381. GRIM, T., O. KLEVEN, AND O. MIKULICA. 2003. Nestling discrimination without recognition: a possible defence mechanism for hosts towards cuckoo parasitism. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 270:S73-S75. HAUBER, M. E. AND R. M. KILNER. 2007. Coevolution, communication, and host-chick mimicry in parasitic finches: who mimics whom? Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 61:497-503. HONZA, M., L. POLACIKOVA, AND P. PROCHAZKA. 2007. Ultraviolet and green parts of the colour spectrum affect egg rejection in the Song Thrush (Turdus philomelos). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 92:269-276. KILNER, R. M. AND N. B. DAVIES. 1998. Nestling mouth colour: ecological correlates of a begging signal. Animal Behaviour 56:705-712. LANGMORE, N. E., S. HUNT, AND R. M. KILNER. 2003. Escalation of a coevolutionary arms race through host rejection of brood parasitic young. Nature 422:156-160. LANGMORE, N. E., G. MAURER, G. J. ADCOCK, AND R. M. KILNER. 2008. Socially acquired host-specific mimicry and the evolution of host races in Horsfield's Bronze-cuckoo Chalcites basalis. Evolution 62:1689-1699. LANGMORE, N. E., A. COCKBURN, A. F. RUSSELL, AND R. M. KILNER. 2009. Flexible cuckoo chick-rejection rules in the Superb Fairy-wren. Behavioral Ecology 20:978-984. LANGMORE, N. E., M. STEVENS, G. MAURER, R. HEINSOHN, M. L. HALL, A. PETERS, AND R. M. KILNER. 2011. Visual mimicry of host nestlings by cuckoos. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 278:2455-2463. LICHTENSTEIN, G. 2001. Low success of Shiny Cowbird chicks parasitizing Rufous-bellied Thrushes: chickchick competition or parental discrimination? Animal Behaviour 61:401-413. LOISEAU, C., S. FELLOUS, C. HAUSSY, O. CHASTEL, AND G. SORCI. 2008. Condition-dependent effects of corticosterone on a carotenoid-based begging signal in House Sparrows. Hormones and Behavior 53:266-273. MARTIN, T. E. AND G. R. GEUPEL. 1993. Nest-monitoring plots: methods for locating nests and monitoring success. Journal of Field Ornithology 64:507-519. NICOLAI, J. 1974. Mimicry in parasitic birds. Scientific American 231:92-98. PAYNE, R. B. 2005. Nestling mouth markings and colors of Old World finches Estrildidae: mimicry and coevolution of nesting finches and their Vidua brood parasites. Miscellaneous Publication Number 194. Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, USA. RALPH, C. J., G. R. GEUPEL, P. PYLE, T. E. MARTIN, AND D. S. DESANTE. 1993. Handbook of field methods for monitoring landbirds. USDA, Forest Service, General Technical Report PSW-GTR 144. Pacific Southwest Research Station, Albany, California, USA. REDONDO, T. 1993. Exploitation of host mechanisms for parental care by avian brood parasites. Etologia 3: 235-297. ROTHSTEIN, S. I. 1978. Geographical variation in nesting coloration of parasitic cowbirds. Auk 95:152-160. SAINO, N., R. AMBROSINI, R. MARTINELLI, P. NINNI, AND A. P. MOLLER. 2003. Gape coloration reliably reflects immunocompetence of Barn Swallow (Hirundo rustica) nestlings. Behavioral Ecology 14:16-22. SAINO, N., P. NINNI, S. CALZA, R. MARTINELL1, F. DE BERNARDI, AND A. P. MOLLER. 2000. Better red than dead: carotenoids-based mouth colouration reveals infection in Barn Swallow nestlings. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 267:57-61. SATO, N. J., K. TOKUE, R. A. NOSKE, O. K. MIKAMI, AND K. UEDA. 2010. Evicting cuckoo nestlings from the nest: a new anti-parasitism behaviour. Biology Letters 6:67-69. SCHUETZ, J. G. 2005a. Low survival of parasitic chicks may result from their imperfect adaptation to hosts rather than expression of defenses against parasitism. Evolution 59:2017-2024. SCHUETZ, J. G. 2005b. Reduced growth but not survival of chicks with altered gape patterns: implications for the evolution of nestling similarity in a parasitic finch. Animal Behaviour 70:839-848. SHIZUKA, D. AND B. E. LYON. 2010. Coots use hatch order to learn to recognize and reject conspecific brood parasitic chicks. Nature 463:223-226. SOLER, M. 2008. Do hosts of interspecific brood parasites feed parasitic chicks with lower-quality prey? Animal Behaviour 76:1761-1763. STEVENS, M., A. PARRAGA, I. C. CUTHILL, J. C. PARTRIDGE, AND T. S. TROSCIANKO. 2007. Using digital photography to study animal coloration. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 90:211-237. THOROGOOD, R., R. M. KILNER, F. KARADA, AND J. G. EWEN. 2008. Spectral mouth colour of nestlings changes with carotenoid availability. Functional Ecology 22:1044-1051. TOKUE, K. AND K. UEDA. 2010. Mangrove Gerygones.Gerygone laevigaster eject Little Bronze-cuckoo Chalcites minutillus hatchlings from parasitized nests. Ibis 152:835-839. TONRA, C. M., M. D. JOHNSON, S. K. HEATH, AND M. E. HAUBER. 2009. Does nesting habitat predict hatch synchrony between brood parasitic Brown-headed Cowbirds Molothrus ater and two hosts? Ecography 32:1-7. REBECCA CROSTON, (1,7,8) CHRISTOPHER M. TONRA, (2,5) SACHA K. HEATH, (2,3,6) AND MARK E. HAUBER (1,4) (1) Graduate Program in Biology, Ecology, Evolutionary Biology and Behavior Subprogram, Graduate Center, City University of New York, NY 10016, USA. (2) Department of Wildlife, Humboldt State University, Arcata, CA 95521, USA. (3) pRBO Conservation Science, Petaluma, CA 94954, USA. (4) Department of Psychology, Hunter College, City University of New York, NY 10065, USA. (5) Current address: Smithsonian Conservation Biology Institute, National Zoological Park, P. O. Box 37012-MRC 5503, Washington, D. C. 20013, USA. (6) Current address: Department of Wildlife, Humboldt State University, Arcata, CA 95521, USA. (7) Department of Psychology, Hunter College, 695 Park Avenue, New York, NY 10065, USA. (8) Corresponding author; e-mail: RCroston@gc.cuny.edu TABLE 1. Eigenvalues from the principal components analysis of variation in nestling flange color. The first two principal components explained 97.4% of the total variance. Parameter Eigenvalue Eigenvector Variance explained (%) Principal Component I 2.51 red 0.58 83.7 green 0.61 blue 0.54 Principal Component II 0.41 red -0.59 13.7 green -0.15 blue 0.79 Principal Component III 0.08 red 0.56 2.7 green -0.78 blue 0.28

back    top    next   
Online Service
Skype Chat Now on Skype! Company QQ Chat Now on QQ! WhatsApp +8613969085317 WeChat

Add Wechat ID

Telephone
0086-531-83297728-8001
+86-156 6830 0963
Fax
0086-531-83297728-8007
E-Mail
sale@hyupshin.cn

╱╲
TOP